Wednesday, September 23, 2015

Interesting interview with Cori Bargmann

Cori Bargmann is the Chair of the  advisory commission for the Brain Initiative.

She studies worms.


Here's the comment I just posted on my Facebook page (which should not be directly accessible to you):

She's a good scientist, but misleads when she says that studying the worm brain might help understand the human brain. It might, but already it's clear that it probably won't answer any of the important questions, like how does the cortex work? Essentially she's looking for her keys where the light is, not in the dark place she dropped them. Of course there may be interesting objects under the lamp-post - even a million dollars which would buy a good locksmith, but it's rather unlikely. To understand the human brain you have to study the cortex directly: try to find the out what the circuits are and what they do, and above all whether there are general principles that underlie the observed variations which specifically explain the features we are most interested in (eg intelligence). Of course evolution does manifest a type of intelligence, and there may be interesting deep connections with neural intelligence.

Sunday, September 13, 2015

Videos and Discussion of Potassium Channel Permeation and Selectivity



The above videos should be helpful in understanding the structure and function of the basic type of potassium channel, the "inward rectifier". My lecture and Notes describe the basic features of selective permeation, especially the  narrowest part of the open pore the "selectivity filter". This is lined with 20 (5X4) carbonyl  groups, all pointing to the interior. These are part of the backbone of the polypeptides, and the corresponding amino acid side chains point outward and interact with the rest of the protein, so the carbonyls are held rigidly, and cannot move inward to better contact sodium ions. Therefore these ions cannot lower their energy enough to compensate for that gained by losing H20 "hydration" molecules as they squeeze into the narrow pore. But K+ ions can lower their energy and enter the narrow part of the pore, and then easily move from site to site (e.g s1 to s2 or s3 to s2), depending on whether adjacent sites are already occupied by a K ion. Because the narrow part of the pore (the "filter") always has 2 ions (otherwise the complex structure collapses, eg at very low K cocentration) the only movements we need consider are between the 2 possible states 1,3 or 2,4. Both can occur, but the relative numbers depend on factors such as K concentration on both sides of the membrane, Vm and the chemical energies of the ions in different states. While I diagrammed a simplified "chemical energy" diagram, which showed only equally low energy wells, with the ion pair are located at the lowest energy positions, of course the ions are moving and the total chemical energy depends at any time depends on state and position. It's the complete energy profile for all possible combinations of state (though to a good approximation only movements between  the 1,3 and 2/4 states are important and position that actually determine the movements of ions and thus the unidirectional and net potassium currents (more about this in a later lecture).
Crucially the membrane potential affects the energies at all positions. A reasonable approximation  is to assume the field is constant at all positions (this "constant field" assumption is used in deriving the GHK membrane equation referred to in the lecture on membrane potential), i.e. that the voltage changes linearly with position (though perhaps most of the potential drop occurs along the narrow filter, so we would first consider the situation at zero Vm, then at other Vms. (I will post diagrams for this)

Notice one crucial point: whatever the current at various Vms and K external/internal concentrations we know that the net K current at the K Nernst potential must be zero, and our quantative analysis must yield this result.
THe main point of this discussion that embodying our recent understanding of molecular structural details can take us far beyond the simple pictures we used before (either Ohm's law or electrodiffusion across a uniform membrane as in the GHK picture). However it can get complicated , and these more detailed models don't really affect the basic conclusions we reached previously: qualitaively Ik = Gk(Vm-Ek). They do offer a more complete picture of ion channel function, and you should know the gist of the ideas involved.

SUMMARY: the movement of K ions through the selectivity filter can be modeled as cycling between 1,3 and 2/4 states. Inic current occurs when there are move cycles in 1 direction than the other. When the pore moves form the first state to the second, an ion has moved inward, and vice versa. If the concentration difference on either side is zero, and Vm is too, then clearly there cannot be net cycling either way. If Kout/Kin is > 1, with Vm = 0, there will be net inward cycling; if K out/Kin = 1 bit Vm is < 0, there will also be net inward cycling. If Vm = Ek (the Nernst potential) there is no net cycling  though neither Vm = 0 nor Ko/Ki = 1. In general knowing the energy profile for various pore occupancy states allows one to calculate Ik under any conditions.

Tuesday, September 8, 2015

Breaking News : Lasker Prize just awarded for research on DNA repair: the "sloppier copier"

Evelyn Witkin just won the Lasker Prize (often a prelude the Nobel) for her pioneering work on DNA repair  . As I explained in my lecture, the polymerase  that quickly repairs damaged DNA (eg by UV light) is a "sloppier copier" - it has to work very quickly to copy the short error-free (parental) strand, after removing the stretch that contains the damage. To do this fast, it dispenses with proofreading, which of course greatly increases the mutation rate, and can lead to cancer - a disease that's closely related to the Eigen "error catastrophe".

see http://bcove.me/a7l91kqj for a nice video on mismatch repair (which is repair of replicative errors).

Response to a student's question re Eigen's model

The population of the master sequence as a fraction of the total population (n) as a function of overall mutation rate (1-Q). The total number of digits per sequence is L=100, and the master sequence has a selective advantage of a=1.05. The "phase transition" is seen to occur at roughly 1-Q=0.05. From https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Error_threshold_(evolution) 

NOTE: the ordinate (Y-axis) is on a log scale, so the almost all-or none change in the concentration of the master is by a factor around 10^28 (ten to the twenty eight, a million times greater than Avogadro's number. 


Here is part of the student's interpretation of my lecture on Eigen's model, and my response, in bold. More to follow....




"The Eigen model of molecular evolution allows us to make a connection between darwinian evolution and the origins of DNA/ RNA replication. Eigen's model works on the basis that in our universes early history, RNA were able to fold and function as catalysts for Polypeptide replication before DNA came into the picture. When DNA developed, the process changed slightly, including the creation of complimentary strands before the identical strand is produced. "


Not quite right. Eigen's model does not explicitly aim to model conditions on the early earth, nor to describe the special case of RNA replication. Indeed at the time the model was first developed (1970), the catalytic abiilities of RNA were not yet recognized. Eigen's model had a more general aim: to describe quantitatively a simplified and generalized model of polynucleotide (or indeed heteropolymer) replication. The first core feature is sequence copying - that the precise linear sequence of n monomers in a polymer made up of at least 2 different monomers (for convenience represented as 0 or 1) depends in a 1-to-1, 0 to 0 fashion on the precise linear sequence of another "template" sequence. All other specifics are left out. As I explained, we must consider the relative concentration of all possible 2^n possible sequences when each sequence is growing exponentially at different rates phi but also "killed" (eg by simple dilution) with equal probability (because every so often half the solution is thrown away, so as to keep the total number of molecules constant despite the ongoing replication. 

So 2 key features of the model are sequence-specific replication and competition for resources (i.e. hi-energy nucleotides). The third crucial feature is the possibility of mistakes in copying individual monomers (eg bases) despite the high specificity of e.g Crick-Watson base pairing. This is a minimal model of Darwinian evolution at the molecular (not organismal) level. Eigen suspected, and was able to prove (both mathematically and experimentally - a nice combination) that if the error rate e exceeded a critical value approximately equal to 1/n, that Darwinian natural selection would stop. Or, applied to the Origin of Life problem, that since on the early earth replication (i.e. template-dependent copying) was probably rather inaccurate, Darwinian evolution could not start until the relevant catalyst (whatever it was) became accurate enough that e < 1/n. The whole point is that at least in the model there is a sharp dividing line (analogous to a phase transition) between purely chemical processes (eg low-accuracy copying) and Darwinian evolution (= Life) at a critical error rate ~ 1/n.   

Note that while the model explicitly considers only sequences of fixed length n exactly the same outcome would be observed in a model with variable length n (for example, in the likely case that shorter sequences were more likely than longer ones). The model throws out all the interesting but basically irrelevant details to focus on the essence of the problem: point mutation.


Thursday, September 3, 2015

Order and Disorder: who to marry

In yesterday's lecture I briefly discussed the connection between 2 apparently different types of disordering process: thermal agitation (= temperature) in phase transitions such as melting of ice, boiling of water and loss of magnetism, and mutation in molecular evolution. The main type of mutation is "point mutation", the occurrence of incorrect Crick-Watson base-pairing (i.e. other than A-G or C-T). The latter results from the fact that the difference in the free energy change (in water) that accompanies correct or incorrect pairing ("delta E") is not infinite (in fact it's only around 2 kcal/mole*)  - hydrogen bonding can occur between incorrect pairs (e.g A-A or A-C), though not as snugly as for correct pairing.

The Boltzmann equation states:

Phi/Plo = exp -(deltaE/kT)

where phi/lo are the (mutually exclusive) probabilities of being in hi energy versus lower energy states and kT is the thermal energy (0.6 kcal/mole). Here we can interpret the hi energy state as incorrect pairing. Inserting the above energy values we get error rate = exp- 3.4 ~ 0.03 i.e. around 3%!
As discussed by Kunkel DNA polymerases can achieve an error rate approaching 10^-10 using 3 combined, strategies: active site geometry (e.g. exclusion of water),  proofreading and mismatch repair.

Obviously thermal agitation (i.e. T)  plays a crucial role in mutation. That's why a man's testicles hang down from the body: it's cooler, though more vulnerable and less elegant than the female arrangement. Note that while I argued (in the Notes and the Lecture) that the high human intergeneration mutation rate favored rapid evolution (broadening of the quasi-species), at the individual selection level (mate choice) it always pays to choose the younger man, who has the lowest mutation rate (other things being equal, which they rarely are).

After the lecture a student asked if this idea (the battle between order and disorder) would be the major theme of the course. I replied, somewhat hastily, that once we got into the brain, this would not be a major theme - we will usually assume synapses, neurons and the brain work perfectly. But this was not quite right: we will see that thermal agitation underlies diffusion which powers the brain's batteries, and that non-thermal phenomena can cause disorder, or at least total confusion! More to the point, the conceptual/quantitative approach I introduce in the early part of the course will often crop up again as we become neural (and some of these ideas will have direct applications, e.g. in neural networks).
................................................................................................................................................................
*I multiplied energies be molecule  by N, Avogadro's number (6X 10^23, meaning 6 times ten to the power twenty three), converting kT to RT. In other words, one can use either energies per molecule or energies per mole in the Boltzmann formula, but one must be consistent so N cancels on top and bottom)